The strange science of odour memory
, and ,
Smell is a powerful sense. It can , and . Certain odours can even prime people to have .
Odours provide a richness to our perception of the world. But, despite the ubiquity of smell, we understand less about smell memory than we do about visual and auditory memory.
The classic example of smell memory is what has become known as a Proustian memory (or involuntary memory). For this phenomenon, mere exposure to a stimulus can automatically trigger a strong memory from the past. For Proust, it was tea-soaked madeleine that activated a detailed memory of his aunt鈥檚 house.
As a researcher of odour memory, people often tell me stories of smells that triggered vivid autobiographical memories. This might be the smell of hospital food, a certain alcoholic drink or the shampoo of a former lover. This strong relationship between odour and emotion is thought to result from the part of the brain involved in processing odours being positioned within the limbic system 鈥 an area of the brain integral to emotion.
Testing short-term memory for smells
Not all smell is stored in long-term memory, though. Some smells are only retained in memory for short periods. Imagine you鈥檙e shopping for a new aftershave or perfume. You wouldn鈥檛 smell two products at the same time as it would be difficult to distinguish between the two. To decide which one you prefer, you need to smell them one after the other. This means you have to temporarily store the smell and then recall it to make a comparison. We have been examining how people store odours in short-term memory and the extent to which odour memory works differently from other types of memory.
The simplest explanation is that people perform smell memory tasks by verbally labelling the odours (for example: 鈥渟mells like cheese鈥). But using this kind of verbal strategy results in the memory task being a test of verbal rather than olfactory memory, because we鈥檙e storing the word 鈥渃heese鈥 in verbal memory not the actual smell of cheese in odour memory. As researchers, we can limit the use of this strategy by selecting odours that are hard to name. For example, non-food odours are typically harder to label.
Another trick we use is asking participants to repeat words that are irrelevant to the task during the test; this is called 鈥渃oncurrent articulation鈥. Concurrent articulation disrupts the participant鈥檚 ability to name the odours and their ability to silently rehearse the names during the task. For example, if you鈥檙e repeating 鈥渢he, the, the鈥 while sniffing something that smells like new-mown grass, you won鈥檛 be able to store the words 鈥渘ew-mown grass鈥 in your verbal memory. It鈥檚 a bit like trying to read a book while listening to the news.
It has been shown that people can perform short-term olfactory memory tasks , and when . These findings suggest that while verbal labelling can improve the memory for an odour, people are also able to store the actual odour within memory. This is supported by that different parts of the brain are activated when remembering easy-to-name and hard-to-name odours; specifically, the inferior frontal gyrus and the piriform cortex, respectively.
One method by which olfactory short-term memory has been compared with other types of memory is by examining how well people can remember a list of odours. Depending on the specifics of the memory task, people are typically good at remembering the first and last item on a list (a phenomena known as primacy and recency). There is some that, for some tasks, smell memory produces different primacy and recency effects to that of other stimuli. These differences might indicate that your smell memory works in a different way to other types of memory.
Smell memory as a diagnostic tool
You might, quite reasonably, ask why you should be interested in testing smell memory, since most of the time we use our olfactory perception to make judgements about odours (that smell is nice/horrible). But that an impaired sense of smell memory is a predictor of .
To further emphasise this link, (a genetic risk factor for developing Alzheimer鈥檚), who show no signs of dementia, have impaired odour identification. These findings suggest that an olfactory memory test could potentially be used as part of the armoury in detecting the early stages of dementia. Early detection is important, as the earlier the intervention, the better the outcome.
, Senior Lecturer in Psychology, and , PhD Student in Cognitive Psychology,
This article was originally published on . Read the .